Clinical and Experimental Pediatrics

Search

Search

Close


Warning: fopen(/home/virtual/pediatrics/journal/upload/ip_log/ip_log_2024-11.txt) [function.fopen]: failed to open stream: Permission denied in /home/virtual/pediatrics/journal/ip_info/view_data.php on line 93

Warning: fwrite(): supplied argument is not a valid stream resource in /home/virtual/pediatrics/journal/ip_info/view_data.php on line 94

All issues > Volume 66(11); 2023

Sarisuta, Chunsuwan, Hansakunachai, and Sritipsukho: Attention-deficit/hyperactive-impulsive disorder symptoms among grade 1 students with reading disorder in Thailand

Attention-deficit/hyperactive-impulsive disorder symptoms among grade 1 students with reading disorder in Thailand

Patcharapun Sarisuta, MD1, Issarapa Chunsuwan, MD2, Tippawan Hansakunachai, MD2, Paskorn Sritipsukho, MD3
Corresponding author: Patcharapun Sarisuta, MD. Department of Pediatrics, Panyananthapikkhu Chonprathan Medical Center, Srinakharinwirot University, Nonthaburi 11120, Thailand Email: patcharapun@g.swu.ac.th
Received June 2, 2023       Revised August 13, 2023       Accepted August 14, 2023
Abstract
Background
Background
Reading disorder is the most common comorbid condition with attention-deficit/hyperactive-impulsive disorder (ADHD) in school-aged children.
Purpose
Purpose
This study compared symptoms of ADHD among children in grade 1 at risk of reading disorder with children not at risk.
Methods
Methods
This cross-sectional study included 703 students in grade 1 aged 6–8 years from 8 schools in Pathumthani Province, Thailand, in 2019. Reading disorder was assessed using tools developed by Vibulpatanavong and Evans for Thai teachers, whereas the Thai parent and teacher versions of the Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham IV Rating Scale (SNAP-IV) was used to evaluate ADHD symptoms. Demographic data were collected from parents using a self-reported questionnaire.
Results
Results
Among the 703 students with almost equal number of male (n=350) and female (n=353), and the average age of 6.56±0.57 years, 95 (13.51%) were classified (significantly male) at risk of reading disorder. The mean SNAP-IV scores of children with reading disorder reported by parents and teachers (20.23±10.95 and 20.75±15.08, respectively) were significantly higher than those of neurotypical children (16.04 ±8.59 and 9.00±10.14, respectively, P<0.05). Of the 95 students with reading disorder reported by parents and teachers, 29 (30.53%) and 20 (21.05%) respectively, were defined as having ADHD according to the standard cutoff SNAP-IV scores, which were significantly higher than 608 neurotypical students at 108 (17.76%) and 20 (5.59%) (P<0.05). The odds ratios of children with reading disorder having ADHD symptoms according to teacher reports were 3.32 (95% confidence interval [CI], 1.14–9.67; P<0.05), 3.75 (95% CI, 1.60–8.79; P<0.05), and 4.41 (95% CI, 1.20–16.15; P<0.05) for inattentive, hyperactive, and combined presentations, respectively.
Conclusion
Conclusion
Grade 1 students with reading disorder had a significantly higher prevalence of ADHD symptoms than neurotypical students. Therefore, children with reading disorders should undergo ADHD assessments and receive proactive intervention.
Key message
Graphical abstract. RD, reading disorder; SNAP-IV, Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham IV Rating Scale; ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactive-impulsive disorder.
Introduction
Introduction
Specific learning disorders (SLDs) are theoretically the neurodevelopmental disorders causing the impairments of learning abilities, the prevalence rates worldwide of which were reported to be approximately 4%–10% with males 3 to 4 times more suffered than females [1]. SLD is classified into 3 types, i.e., reading disorder, writing disorder, and mathematic disorder, approximately 80% of which is reading disorder [2,3]. The screening number and prevalence rate in Thailand conducted by the Ministry of Education in 2009 in 9,828 schools, which is approximately 1/3 of the total number of schools all over the country, was found to be 15.6% at risk of SLD [4].
Attention-deficit/hyperactive-impulsive disorder (ADHD) is one of the most common disorders in school-aged children, which is characterized by excessive activity, short attention span, and impulsivity, with childhood-onset and at least partial persistence into adulthood in the majority of cases [1,5]. ADHD is classified into 3 different presentations by Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition (DSM-5) (formerly known as subtypes in DSM-IV), i.e., predominantly inattentive, predominantly hyperactive/impulsive, and combined presentations [1,6]. The recent advances from neuroimaging as well as frontal volume have improved our understanding of the neurobiology of ADHD which is theoretically the disorders of nervous system [7,8]. Although the prevalence rates worldwide of ADHD in school children are considerably varied and estimated to be between 2% and 10% of the population, it is considered to be underdiagnosed [5]. A more recent study in 7,188 Thai students graded 1–5 in primary school revealed the prevalence of 8.1% with the ratio of boy to girl equal to 3:1. Besides, the prevalence of subtype in ADHD was mostly of the combined type of 3.8% and hyperactive/impulsive type were lowest at 0.9% [9].
ADHD is a highly comorbid condition associated with other neurological and psychiatric disorders. Reading disorder seems to coexist most clearly with ADHD [10]. Of all psychological comorbidities of reading disorder, ADHD was reported to be the most common comorbid disorder about 18%–42% [2,3,11]. A recent study also supports the notion that reading problems and dyslexia are more clearly associated with inattention than the hyperactivity/impulsivity dimension of ADHD and this association is genetically mediated [12]. Up to date, it is not clear, on which exact functional processes this comorbidity is based. Some researchers explained that inattention problem from ADHD may lead to learning difficulties while others proposed that children with reading disorder had difficulty in learning in the classroom and lead to symptoms of ADHD [3,13]. Several theories have been offered to explain the profile of the comorbidity group vs. each disorder separately in an attempt to crystalize the characteristics of each condition. Two prominent theories are: (1) cognitive subtype hypothesis - ADHD + reading disorder has a distinct causal mechanism and could be regarded as an independent disorder [14,15] and (2) phenocopy hypothesis - the deficit in ADHD + reading disorder is ADHD with secondary symptoms of reading disorder and therefore, ADHD + reading disorder is not considered as a separate disorder [16]. And another theory was that some brain function abnormalities may be the underlying cause of both reading disorder and ADHD [17-19].
Although the prevalence and problems of SLD have been considerably studied, the exploration of reading disorder, especially for students at the elementary school, i.e., the first phase of enrollment in the compulsory education system, is still very limited. It is thus of interest to explore the prevalence of ADHD in grade 1 school children who were at risk of reading disorder as well as to figure out which subtype of ADHD predominant presentation in grade 1 school children who were at risk of reading disorder. Besides, it is expected that knowing the relationship with reading disorder and ADHD would lead to appropriate early intervention.
Methods
Methods
1. Participants
1. Participants
A cross-sectional study was conducted on grade 1 students in Pathumthani Province, Thailand in 2019. The students included in this study were those aged between 6–8 years, being communicable using Thai, and having been given consent by parents. The excluded students were those previously diagnosed for vision problems, i.e., blindness or blurred vision; listening problems, i.e., deafness or hearing impairments; and developmental problems, i.e., autism, delayed development or intellectual disability.
A sample of 450 students was calculated to detect the effect of reading disorder on ADHD with the power of 80% and P value <0.05. The prevalence of ADHD in general population and children with reading disorder were estimated at 5% (2%–5%) [5], and 18% (18%–42%) [2], respectively.
2. Procedures
2. Procedures
Schools from 3 affiliations (Office of Basic Education, Local Administration Organization, and private schools) were selected by purposive sampling method; schools were located nearby Thammasat University Hospital and were chosen with respect to teachers’ interest and willingness to cooperate. The school directors were then contacted and permission was requested to carry out studies in the schools by providing documents to clarify the research participants. Students had their reading skills assessed face-to-face at schools. Each reading test was performed by a single tester to test all students in a quiet room at the school. The demographic questionnaires and Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham IV Scale (SNAP-IV) parent version were sent out to be completed by parents and returned within one month, whereas the SNAPIV teacher version was completed by homeroom teachers.
3. Assessment of reading skills
3. Assessment of reading skills
The students’ reading skills were assessed by the tools developed by Vibulpatanavong and Evans [20]. The tools were designed for Thai teachers to identify students with reading difficulties and provide assistance from early school years. The word and passage reading tests of the tools were used. The word reading test consists of a total of 60 Thai words in a sequence from easy to difficult, through which the student has to read aloud within 1 minute and be scored for the correct words. The passage reading test comprises 3 prose-style short stories each with the length of 120–175 words. The correct words of each story the students can read within 1 minute are scored and then summed into the total score of the 3 stories. The criterion for students at risk of reading disorder is defined as a sum score of either the word reading or the 3-passage reading which is <10 percentile of the sampled population.
4. Assessment of ADHD
4. Assessment of ADHD
The assessment of ADHD was carried out by using the short form of SNAP-IV, questionnaire, Thai version for parents and homeroom teachers [21,22]. This instrument consists of a total of 26 questions comprising attention deficit 9 questions (No. 1–9), hyperactive-impulsive disorders 9 questions (No. 10–18), and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) 8 questions (No. 19–26). The scoring scale is as follows: 0 for none, 1 for low, 2 for moderate, and 3 for high frequency of symptoms. SNAP-IV has high accuracy with Cronbach alpha values of 0.93–0.96 and sensitivity of approximately 50%–60%.
The original version of SNAP-IV defines the cut-point criteria of the teacher version for identifying children with ADHD as predominantly inattentive, predominantly hyperactive-impulsive presentations, and ODD at scores 23, 16, and 11, respectively, while the cut-point criteria of the parent version are 16, 13, and 15, respectively. Pityarastian et al. [21] conducted a study to determine the appropriate cut-point criteria for the Thai version of SNAP-IV and suggested that scores 18, 11, and 8 of the teacher form and scores 16, 14, and 12 of the parent form were the proper cut-point criteria for defining children with ADHD as predominantly inattentive, predominantly hyperactive-impulsive presentations, and ODD, respectively.
5. Demographic data
5. Demographic data
Demographic data of students about age, sex, parental education, family income, attending kindergarten, delayed speech history, family history of reading or writing difficulty, parents’ perception of their child’s reading ability, and school affiliation were collected from parents by a self-report questionnaire. These data of students at risk of reading disorder and their parents were statistically compared with nonreading disorder groups. Parental education was categorized as high school or lower versus vocational college or higher. Family income was categorized as <285, 285–855, 855–1,425, 1,425–2,850, and >2,850 USD. The previous records whether students having ever attended kindergarten, as well as former delayed speech problem before attending school were collected. The students’ family history of reading or writing difficulty were also compiled.
6. Data analysis
6. Data analysis
Data were analyzed by using Stata 14 (StataCorp LLC, College Station, TX, USA). Descriptive statistics were used to describe demographic data. In the case of categorical data, percentage amount was employed. Mean and standard deviation were used for continuous data. In comparison of data between groups chisquare and Fisher exact test were used in the cases of categorical data, whereas t test was used for continuous data, with P value <0.05 being statistically significant. The independent t test was used for studying the differences between the 2 groups in terms of ADHD symptoms scores. All tests were 2-tailed and considered significant at P value <0.05. In analysis of various influential factors by controlling the effects of other relevant factors, multivariate regression analysis was used. Logistic regression model was used to analyze the relationship between ADHD classification and children with or without risk of reading disorder. The variables such as sex, maternal education, average family income per month, delayed speech history, reading or writing difficulty in the family, parents’ perception of child’s reading ability, and school affiliations were used in a model for adjusting of confounding factors.
7. Ethical approval
7. Ethical approval
Ethical approval for the current study was granted by the Ethics Committee Set 1 of Thammasat University. The approved number of this project was 203/2019, dated October 20, 2019, and the project number was MTU-EC-PE-4-111/19.
Results
Results
1. Prevalence of reading disorder
1. Prevalence of reading disorder
A total of 703 questionnaires with almost equal numbers of male and female students and the average age of 6.56±0.57 years were answered by parents and teachers. There were 95 students or 13.5% at risk of reading disorder based on the assessment of reading skills in this study, as shown by demographic data in Table 1. Demographic data with statistically significant differences for students at risk of reading disorder compared to nonreading disorder were being male, maternal education lower than college, less average family income, delayed speech history, reading or writing difficulty in the family, parents’ perception of child’s reading ability, and school affiliations. It is interesting that maternal education state was correlated with reading disorder risk factors, while paternal education showed no correlation. It was shown that of the students at risk of reading disorder, 21.0% were studying in schools under the affiliation of the Office of Basic Education, 13.4% under Local Administration Organization, and 9.2% under private schools. Notably, school affiliation was also a significant factor associated with reading disorder. On the other hand, there was no significant difference regarding kindergarten attendance of students. Interestingly, only half of the parents of children at risk of reading disorder correctly evaluated that their child had lower literacy skill compared to their peers.
2. Reading disorder with ADHD
2. Reading disorder with ADHD
Students at risk of reading disorder had significantly higher ADHD scores from the SNAP-IV in every predominant presentation from both parent and teacher reports compared with students without reading difficulty (Table 2). Standard and Thai cut-point criteria of SNAP-IV were used to define students whose scores met the criteria of ADHD as predominantly inattentive, predominantly hyperactive-impulsive, and combined presentations. Fig. 1 illustrates the proportions of students who had been defined as having ADHD by standard and Thai cut-point SNAPIV scores who were at risk of reading disorder compared to those who were not at risk of reading disorder. Based on either standard or Thai cut-point criteria, students defined as having ADHD for all presentations exhibited significantly higher proportions for those at risk of reading disorder than nonreading disorder groups (P<0.05), particularly by teacher reports.
Univariate and multivariate analysis were used to determine odds ratios for various ADHD presentations of students at risk of reading disorder compared with nonreading disorder group reported by parents and teachers. The odds ratios were also adjusted for confounders including sex, maternal education, average family income per month, delayed speech history, reading or writing difficulty in the family, parents’ perception of child’s reading ability, and school affiliations. The unadjusted and adjusted odds ratios of SNAP-IV scores using standard cut-point criteria reported by both parents and teachers are shown in Table 3. The adjusted odds ratios of the teacher and parent reports for predominantly inattentive, predominantly hyperactive-impulsive, and combined presentations were 2.81, 1.54, 2.72 and 3.32, 3.75, 4.41, respectively. The odds ratios of teacher reports displayed significant differences for all ADHD presentations, and so did the reports from parents except for predominantly hyperactive-impulsive presentation.
The results using Thai cut-point criteria (Table 4) were in accordance with those using standard cut-point criteria for teacher reports. Children at risk of reading disorder displayed predominantly inattentive, hyperactive-impulsive, and combined presentations at 7.92, 3.86, and 5.47 times more than typical readers, respectively. On the other hand, parent reports using Thai cut-point criteria showed a significant result only for ADHD predominantly inattentive presentation.
It might generally be concluded that students at risk of reading disorder exhibited significant ADHD symptoms for all presentations, compared with nonreading disorder group in the views of teachers, but only for predominantly inattentive presentation in the views of their parents.
Discussion
Discussion
The major findings of this study revealed that students at risk of reading disorder exhibited significant ADHD symptoms for all presentations, compared with nonreading disorder group in the views of teachers, but only for predominantly inattentive presentation in the views of their parents. Gender, parental education level, average family income, and children’s school affiliation exhibited statistically significant influence on reading disorder.
The number of grade 1 students identified as at risk of reading disorder was 13.51% with significantly higher in boys 2.65 times than in girls, whereas those in nonreading disorder group were not significantly different. These numbers were considerably higher than the prevalence rates of approximately 4%–7% with plenty in males reported in the literature [2,3,5], but were in accordance with the formerly screening number and prevalence rate in Thailand at 15.6% [4]. Approximately 18%–45% of all children with ADHD also reach the clinical definition of reading disorder and are considered as “comorbid for ADHD and reading disorder.” [23,24] We found that Thai children with reading difficulties had more ADHD problems than children without reading disorder. In addition, our study further revealed that grade 1 is usually the earliest time when any relationship between reading problems and ADHD symptoms might be observed as this is when children begin compulsory education in Thailand.
The significant increase in SNAP-IV scores, as reported by parents and teachers for all predominant presentations of ADHD in students at risk of reading disorder as compared to the scores of children without reading disorder, indicated a relationship between ADHD symptoms and reading difficulties in these grade 1 students. When both standard and Thai cut-point criteria were used to see if students met ADHD criteria, there were more students at risk of reading disorder who met cut-point criteria in the predominantly inattentive and hyperactive presentations according to teacher reports. However, in the parental reports, only children with predominantly inattentive presentations were more prevalent in the reading disorder at-risk group. Such results were in accord with the previous reports revealing that ADHD predominantly inattentive and ADHD-combined, but not ADHD predominantly hyperactive-impulsive were associated with lower reading fluency, reading comprehension, and mathematics ability as well as higher reading and writing errors [17,25]. It is likely because teachers observed the students in the classroom, where children with reading difficulties may struggle to sit still and find it challenging to focus on a lesson. For instance, these children, who might be restless and probably walk around the classroom, will undoubtedly be noticed by the teacher. In contrast, parents typically observe their children at home likely running and playing freely so that they might not feel that their children are hyperactive. Nevertheless, parents are likely to notice situations in which the children are unable to concentrate, such as when they are doing their homework, therefore giving higher inattentive scores for these children at risk of reading disorder.
Our study found that gender, parental education level, average family income, and children’s school affiliation exhibited statistically significant influence on reading disorder. These were in accordance with the previous reports stating that lower parental education [26] and low family income [26,27] imposed significant influence on reading disorder of students. It is interesting to note that most of the students at risk of reading disorder were studying in schools affiliated with the Office of Basic Education. This may be due to the fact that children attending these public schools come from families of lower socioeconomic status. Probable factors such as limitations in budget, number of teachers, and teaching materials may affect teaching quality. The finding that reading disorder risk varies depending on school affiliation suggests the educational need for increased support and concern for students of schools with lower affiliations.
Since these variables may be confounding factors for ADHD, we adjusted them to determine if children at risk for reading disorder were still more likely to have ADHD. The results revealed that children with reading disorder were still at a higher risk of having ADHD than children without reading disorder. The odds ratios were between 3–4 times for ADHD predominantly inattentive presentations, and 3–7 times for ADHD predominantly hyperactive presentations according to the teacher reports. The odds ratio of ADHD from parental reports showed that inattentive presentation was 2.8 times greater in children at risk of reading disorder.
There are several hypotheses trying to explain the relationship between ADHD and reading disorder. The phenocopy hypothesis [16] considers ADHD symptoms to be a consequence of reading failure. When children have reading difficulty, they face challenges in following up on lessons and then finally lose interest in classroom learning, leading to ADHD symptoms. On the other hand, inattention problems may lead to reading difficulty. Children with ADHD have difficulty concentrating in the classroom and, thus, have problems learning to read. The cognitive subtype hypothesis [14,15] is that ADHD and reading disorder may derive from the same underlying origin such as a basic brain function impairment for both attention and reading. Our research supports cognitive subtype hypothesis because we found that the proportion of children with reading difficulties who presented with ADHD was greater. They had just enrolled in compulsory education and been taught formal reading lessons for only one semester. Therefore, it seems unlikely that ADHD was a consequence of reading disorder or vice versa. We are more inclined to agree with the hypothesis that ADHD and reading disorder arise from the same cognitive domain weaknesses.
Our research appears to be the first community and schoolbased study in Thailand to determine the relationship between reading problems and ADHD symptoms in children. This is different from previous research in Thailand [28], which had examined the comorbidities of children with SLD in a hospital setting; children with SLD who come to see a hospital specialist usually have a learning disorder or troubling behavioral problems, provoking parents to bring them to see a doctor. Our study builds on other research about children with reading disorder. In a previous report, children who were classified as having reading disorder because they were less able to read words and passages than their classmates (below 10th percentile) also had more ADHD symptoms. Moreover, the relationship between reading problems and ADHD is first observed when children begin to learn to read Thai in line with the national curriculum.
Of note, there is a high probability that children with reading difficulties in grade 1 may not receive systematic assistance here in Thailand as there is still limited screening and assistance for children with SLD within the school system. Our research highlights the need to screen for reading disorder and then support these children at an early stage with assistance for reading skills. It would be helpful to also screen these at-risk children for ADHD.
Footnotes

Conflicts of interest

No potential conflict of interest relevant to this article was reported.

Funding

This work was financially supported by The Health System Research Institute, Ministry of Public Health, Thailand, Thai Clinical Trial Registry: TCTR20191009001.

Author contribution

Conceptualization: P.Sarisuta, IC; Data curation: P.Sritipsukho; Formal analysis: TH; Funding acquisition: P.Sritipsukho; Methodology: P.Sarisuta; Project administration: P.Sritipsukho; Visualization: P.Sarisuta, TH; Writing – original draft: P.Sarisuta; Writing – review & editing: P.Sarisuta, IC

Acknowledgments

Appreciation is given to all students and their parents, as well as directors and homeroom teachers of schools participating in this project.

Fig. 1.
Proportion of students at risk of reading disorder defined as attention-deficit/hyperactivityimpulsivity by standard and Thai cutoff Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham IV Rating Scale scores compared to the nonreading disorder group (N=703; P<0.05 by *Chi-square test. **Fisher exact test). RD, reading disorder.
cep-2023-00773f1.tif
cep-2023-00773f2.tif
Table 1.
Demographic data of students at risk versus not at risk of reading disorder and their parents
Variable Reading disorder (n=95) Nonreading disorder (n=608) Total P value
Sex <0.001
 Female 26 (27.4) 327 (53.8) 353 (50.2)
 Male 69 (72.6) 281 (46.2) 350 (49.8)
Age (yr) 6.52±0.76 6.57±0.53 6.56±0.57 0.523
Maternal education 0.044
 High school or lower 68 (77.3) 387 (66.5) 455 (67.9)
 Vocational college or higher 20 (22.7) 195 (33.5) 215 (32.1)
Paternal education 0.058
 High school or lower 65 (78.3) 385 (68.0) 450 (69.3)
 Vocational college or higher 18 (21.7) 181 (32.0) 199 (30.7)
Average family income per month (USD) 0.030
 <285 30 (33.7) 115 (19.8) 145 (21.6)
 285–855 43 (48.3) 299 (51.5) 342 (51.0)
 855–1,425 10 (11.2) 110 (18.9) 120 (17.9)
 1,425–2,850 4 (4.5) 45 (7.8) 49 (7.3)
 >2,850 2 (2.3) 12 (2.1) 14 (2.1)
Kindergarten school 0.135
 Attend 88 (97.8) 580 (99.5) 668 (99.3)
 Not attend 2 (2.2) 3 (0.5) 5 (0.7)
 Unknown 2 4 6
Delayed speech history <0.001
 Yes 13 (15.8) 21 (3.8) 34 (5.4)
 No 69 (84.2) 533 (96.2) 602 (94.6)
 Unknown 2 (2.2) 8 10
Family history of reading or writing difficulty 0.001
 Yes 21 (22.1) 62 (10.2) 83 (11.8)
 No 74 (77.9) 546 (89.8) 620 (88.2)
 Unknown 8 21 29
Parents’ perception of their child’s reading ability <0.001
 Inferior 37 (55.2) 46 (8.9) 83 (14.3)
 Normal 29 (43.3) 433 (84.1) 462 (79.4)
 Superior 1 (1.5) 36 (7.0) 37 (6.3)
 Unknown 24 73 97
School affiliation 0.001
 Office of Basic Education 34 (35.8) 124 (20.4) 158 (22.5)
 Local Administration Organization 34 (35.8) 219 (36.0) 253 (36.0)
 Private schools 27 (28.4) 265 (43.6) 292 (41.5)

Values are presented as number (%) or mean±standard deviation.

USD, United States dollar.

Students at risk of reading disorders were defined as those whose sum score of the word reading or the 3-passage reading test was ≤10th percentile of the sampled population.

Some students' parents did not intend to provide information about their education, incomes, attending kindergarten school, etc., so, these categories are absent in the questionnaires.

Boldface indicates a statistically significant difference with P<0.05.

t test.

Fisher exact test.

Table 2.
Mean SNAP-IV scores of students at risk versus not at risk of reading disorder assessed by parents and teachers (N=703)
ADHD predominantly presentations Reading disorder Nonreading disorder P value
Parent report
 Inattentive 11.26±6.02 8.79±4.61 <0.001
 Hyperactive-impulsive 8.97±5.70 7.25±4.80 0.005
 Total score 20.23±10.95 16.04±8.59 0.001
Teacher report
 Inattentive 13.34±8.28 5.64±5.96 <0.001
 Hyperactive-impulsive 7.41±8.09 3.35±5.03 <0.001
 Total score 20.75±15.08 9.00±10.14 <0.001

Values are presented as mean±standard deviation.

The data were analyzed using an independent t test.

SNAP-IV, Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham IV Rating Scale; ADHD, attentiondeficit/hyperactive-impulsive disorder.

Students at risk of reading disorders were defined as those whose sum score of the word reading or the 3-passage reading test was ≤10th percentile of the sampled population.

Boldface indicates a statistically significant difference with P<0.05.

Table 3.
Logistic regression analysis of ADHD presentations in students at risk versus not at risk of reading disorder using standard cutoff SNAP-IV scores
ADHD predominantly presentations Unadjusted
Adjusted
Odds ratio (95% CI) P value Odds ratio (95% CI) P value
Parent report
 Inattentive 3.31 (1.92–5.73) 2.81 (1.45–5.50) 0.002
 Hyperactive-impulsive 1.64 (0.95–2.84) 0.072 1.54 (0.82–2.90) 0.177
 Combined 3.30 (1.67–2.84) 0.001 2.72 (1.19–6.19) 0.017
Teacher report
 Inattentive 6.08 (2.72–13.59) <0.001 3.32 (1.14–9.67) 0.027
 Hyperactive-impulsive 4.83 (2.51–9.32) <0.001 3.75 (1.60–8.79) 0.002
 Combined 8.92 (3.23–24.54) <0.001 4.41 (1.20-16.15) 0.025

With reference to nonreading disorder group.

Adjusted for confounders including sex, maternal education, average monthly family income, delayed speech history, family history of reading or writing difficulty, parents’ perception of child’s reading ability, and school affiliations.

ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactive-impulsive disorder; SNAP-IV, Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham IV Rating Scale; CI, confidence interval.

The standard cutoff criteria of the teacher version for children with ADHD were predominantly inattentive, predominantly hyperactive-impulsive presentations, and oppositional defiant disorder were 23, 16, and 11, respectively, while those of the parent version were 16, 13, and 15, respectively.

Boldface indicates a statistically significant difference with P<0.05.

Table 4.
Logistic regression analysis of ADHD presentations in students at risk versus not at risk of reading disorder using Thai cutoff SNAP-IV scores
ADHD predominantly presentations Unadjusted
Adjusted
Odds ratio (95% CI) P value Odds ratio (95% CI) P value
Parent report
 Inattentive 3.31 (1.92–5.73) <0.001 2.81 (1.45–5.50) 0.002
 Hyperactive-impulsive 1.71 (0.94–3.10) 0.079 1.47 (0.72–2.97) 0.288
 Combined 2.79 (1.33–5.84) 0.006 1.95 (0.77–4.97) 0.159
Teacher report
 Inattentive 9.48 (5.60 -16.07) <0.001 7.92 (4.03–15.55) <0.001
 Hyperactive-impulsive 4.55 (2.74–7.56) <0.001 3.86 (2.05–7.24) <0.001
 Combined 6.96 (3.74 -12.98) <0.001 5.47 (2.45–12.22) <0.001

With reference to the nonreading disorder group.

Adjusted for confounders including sex, maternal education, average monthly family income, delayed speech history, family history of reading or writing disorder, parents’ perception of child’s reading ability, and school affiliations.

ADHD, attention-deficit/hyperactivity-impulsivity disorder; SNAP-IV, Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham IV Rating Scale; CI, confidence interval.

The Thai cutoff criteria of the teacher version for children with ADHD were predominantly inattentive, predominantly hyperactive-impulsive presentations, and oppositional defiant disorder were 18, 11, and 8, respectively, while those of the parent version were 16, 14, and 12, respectively.

Boldface indicates a statistically significant difference with P<0.05.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Fifth Edition. DSM-5TM. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press, 2013.
2. Germanò E, Gagliano A. Comorbidity of ADHD and dyslexia. Dev Neuropsychol 2010;35:475–93.
[Article] [PubMed]
3. Sexton CC, Gelhorn HL, Bell JA, Classi PM. The co-occurrence of reading disorder and ADHD: epidemiology, treatment, psychosocial impact, and economic burden. J Learn Disabil 2012;45:538–64.
[Article] [PubMed]
4. Pougaksorn S. Annual report summary in national survey for learning disabilities. Bangkok (Thailand): Ministry of Education, 2009.
5. Polyzoi M, Ahnemark E, Medin E, Ginsberg Y. Estimated prevalence and incidence of diagnosed ADHD and health care utilization in adults in Sweden – a longitudinal population-based register study. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat 2018;14:1149–61.
[PubMed] [PMC]
6. Niemczyk J, Equit M, Braun-Bither K, Klein AM, Gontard AV. Prevalence of incontinence, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and oppositional defiant disorder in preschool children. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2015;24:837–43.
[Article] [PubMed]
7. Jagger-Rickels AC, Kibby MY, Constance JM. Global gray matter morphometry differences between children with reading disability, ADHD, and comorbid reading disability/ADHD. Brain Lang 2018;185:54–66.
[Article] [PubMed] [PMC]
8. Kibby MY, Dyer SM, Lee SE, Stacy M. Frontal volume as a potential source of the comorbidity between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and reading disorders. Behav Brain Res 2020;381:112382.
[Article] [PubMed] [PMC]
9. Visanuyothin T, Pavasuthipaisit C, Wachiradilok P, Arunruang P, Buranasuksakul T. The prevalence of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder in Thailand. J Ment Health Thai 2013;21:66–75.
10. Peterson RL, Pennington BF. Developmental dyslexia. Annu Rev Clin Psychol 2015;11:283–307.
[Article] [PubMed]
11. Czamara D, Tiesler CMT, Kohlbock G, Berdel D, Hoffmann B, Bauer CP, et al. Children with ADHD symptoms have a higher risk for reading, spelling and math difficulties in the GINIplus and LISAplus cohort studies. PLoS One 2013;8:e63859.
[Article] [PubMed] [PMC]
12. Mascheretti S, De Luca A, Trezzi V, Peruzzo D, Nordio A, Marino C, et al. Neurogenetics of developmental dyslexia: From genes to behavior through brain neuroimaging and cognitive and sensorial mechanisms. Transl Psychiatry 2017;7:e987.
[Article] [PubMed] [PMC]
13. Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA. Paying attention to reading: the neurobiology of reading and dyslexia. Dev Psychopathol 2008;20:1329–49.
[Article] [PubMed]
14. de Jong CGW, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant JA. The role of double dissociation studies in the search for candidate endophenotypes for the comorbidity of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and reading disability. Int J Disabil Dev Ed 2006;53:177–93.
[Article]
15. Rucklidge JJ, Tannock R. Neuropsychological profiles of adolescents with ADHD: effects of reading difficulties and gender. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2002;43:988–1003.
[Article] [PubMed]
16. Pennington BF, Groisser D, Welsh MC. Contrasting cognitive deficits in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder versus reading disability. Developmental Psychology 1993;29:511–23.
[Article]
17. Ahmed MA, El-Beh KA, Mohammad TA, Mansour DF, Ezz-Eldine MY. Learning disabilities in different types of attention deficit hyperactivity disorders and its relation to cortical and brainstem function. J Neurol Res 2014;4:22–30.
[Article]
18. Livingston EM, Siegel LS, Ribary U. Developmental dyslexia: emotional impact and consequences. Aust J Learn Diffic 2018;23:107–35.
[Article]
19. Lonergan A, Doyle C, Cassidy C, Mahon SM, Roche RAP, Boran L, et al. A meta-analysis of executive functioning in dyslexia with consideration of the impact of comorbid ADHD. J Cogn Psychol 2019;31:725–49.
[Article]
20. Vibulpatanavong K, Evans D. Off to a good start: enhancing reading skills in Thai children. Paper presented at the First International Conference on Special Education, SEAMEO, 2015; 2015 Jul 28th-31th; Bangkok, Thailand.
21. Pityarastian N, Booranasuksakul T, Juengsiragulwit D, Benyakorn S. ADHD screening properties of the Thai version of Swanson, Nolan, Pelham IV Scale (SNAP-IV) and strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ). J Psychiatr Assoc Thai 2014;59:97–110.
22. Swanson J, Lerner M, March J, Gresham FM. Assessment and intervention for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in the schools. Lessons from the MTA study. Pediatr Clin North Am 1999;46:993–1009.
[PubMed]
23. Gnanavel S, Sharma P, Kaushal P, Hussain S. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and comorbidity: a review of literature. World J Clin Cases 2019;7:2420–6.
[Article] [PubMed] [PMC]
24. Horowitz-Kraus T, Hershey A, Kay B, DiFrancesco M. Differential effect of reading training on functional connectivity in children with reading difficulties with and without ADHD comorbidity. J Neurolinguistics 2018;49:93–108.
[Article] [PubMed] [PMC]
25. Öner Ö, Vatanartiran S, Karadeniz S. Relationships between teacherreported ADHD symptom profiles and academic achievement domains in a nonreferred convenience sample of first- to fourth-grade students. Psychiatry Clin Psychopharmacol 2019;29:502–8.
[Article]
26. Duncan GJ, Magnuson K. Socioeconomic status and cognitive functioning: moving from correlation to causation. Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 2012;3:377–86.
[Article] [PubMed]
27. Mascheretti S, Andreola C, Scaini S, Sulpizio S. Beyond genes: a systematic review of environmental risk factors in specific reading disorder. Res Dev Disabil 2018;82:147–52.
[Article] [PubMed]
28. Piyasil V, Wangtan S. Learning disorder and comorbidity. J Psychiatr Assoc Thai 2015;60:287–96.

Go to Top